Click HERE to visit the Little Miracles website and sign up for our mailing list! We'll keep you informed of everything from product recalls to contest giveaways!
Showing posts with label teaching. Show all posts
Showing posts with label teaching. Show all posts

Thursday, August 26, 2010

Babies are teaching scientists much about the human mind

from The Oregonian

infant.JPG

Problems like this arise constantly in life: You hit the "print" command on your computer and nothing happens. To get anywhere, you have to figure out whether the printer is broken or you're just doing something wrong, like forgetting to turn on the power.

Babies, it turns out, possess reasoning skills that make them adept at solving this kind of problem.

In one recent study, 16-month-olds quickly and accurately deduced whether a toy that failed to make music needed to be replaced or that they needed to ask for help because they did something wrong.

"Babies come into the world with a lot of initial knowledge and ways of thinking. They make rich, abstract inferences from just a little bit of evidence. They are the most powerful learners in the universe," says Laura Schulz, a professor in the department of brain and cognitive sciences at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology in Cambridge. She and hundreds of other scientists presented findings in Portland this week at the annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, a group including anthropologists, linguists, neuroscientists, psychologists and others striving to understand how our brains learn, create and make us who we are.

Many focus on the minds of babies, who are proving to have abilities far beyond what researchers long assumed. In the past year or two, studies have shown newborns grasp abstract numbers enough to link matching numbers of objects and sounds, they understand musical rhythm well enough to detect a missed downbeat in a percussion line, and show moral judgment by 3 months.

The mind in infancy acquires more abilities in less time than at any other age, making it an ideal time for studying the process of learning. And infants offer a window on basic workings of the mind, before culture and other experiences multiply the differences between people at older ages.

Delving into abstract reasoning, Schulz and MIT graduate student Hyowon Gweon showed a series of 16-month-olds how to push a button on a green toy to make it play music. In one trial, researchers handed the toy to the baby to try. In another trial, they handed the baby a yellow toy that otherwise looked identical. In both cases, it was impossible for the babies to make the toy play music. The green toy required activating a second, hidden switch while the yellow toy had no music maker.

When the switch failed to work, babies had two options: turn to a parent for help or reach for a third toy, colored red, placed just out of reach. The babies consistently made the right choice. When given the green toy that they had seen working a moment earlier they sought help from a parent. But when given the yellow toy, they assumed it was broken and grabbed for the red toy.

"They are able to attend to very subtle differences in the world," says Gweon.

Other groups are exploring babies' sophisticated moral reasoning. As early as 3 months, newborns show a preference for puppets or animated characters that help others over those that hinder, in one study. Kiley Hamlin and colleagues at Yale University in New Haven, Conn., showed babies scenes with puppets struggling up a hill. Some puppets helped while others pushed their struggling fellows down the hill. When offered a chance to play with the puppets, the infants picked helpers almost 100 percent of the time.

"And they make inferences about how characters will behave in the future," says Hamlin.

When a hindering character acts in a helpful way, infants register surprise by gazing longer.

In a related study presented at the Portland meeting, Korean researchers found that 16-month-olds not only prefer helpful behavior, but they also expect it. Woo-yeol Lee and others from Yonsei University in South Korea showed babies animated cartoons enacting hill-climbing struggle similar to the puppet show. One character, a square, easily climbed a steep hill but a circle character could not. In some scenes, the square helped the circle and in others, the square ignored its struggling fellow. When the square failed to help, babies consistently showed surprise by staring longer at the ignoring event than they did at helping events.

"They very rapidly develop awareness of moral obligation," says Tamar Kushnir, assistant professor and director of the Early Childhood Cognition Laboratory at Cornell University in Ithaca, N.Y. Kushnir and graduate student Nadia Chernyak presented studies on preschoolers' ability to reason about morality and other limits on free will.

They asked 4- and 5-year-olds to draw a picture and gave them two choices of what to draw. A puppet presented the kids with varying pressures to draw one shape and not the other. In some trials, the puppet explained that triangles, for example, were sad and made him cry. In others, the puppet told the children they "have to draw a squiggly," or exerted pressure to conform by saying "all of the other boys and girls" drew a line. One puppet told kids to draw the shape "you like best."

The moral pressure and the pressure to conform showed the strongest effect: Thirteen out of 15 children said they did not have a choice to draw the picture that would make the puppet cry, or to draw a picture other than the one everyone else drew. The children felt freer to ignore orders or to draw a picture other than their favorite.

When asked to explain why their choice was limited, the children were able to do so consistently only in the case of the moral pressure to avoid hurting the puppet. While pressure to conform limited just as many children, only two out of 15 were able to name that reason. The strength and sophistication of moral reasoning surprised researchers.

"It was just whopping," Kushnir says.

-- Joe Rojas-Burke

Wednesday, August 18, 2010

What is Baby Sign Language?

from Nature Mom Blog




Baby sign language is American Sign Language, only cuter. And there’s often some drool thrown in for effect.
Seriously though, American Sign Language is the official language of the deaf community in the United States. You probably learned the American Sign Alphabet at some point in school.

The baby version of American Sign started in the 70’s when Joseph Garcia observed that the hearing babies of deaf parents learned to talk at an earlier age than the hearing babies of hearing parents. This intrigued Garcia, and prompted him to start a program that trained hearing parents to teach their babies sign language.
And Garcia’s original observation held true. Babies who learned to sign learned to speak at an earlier age than those who didn’t learn to sign. But that wasn’t all. Many other, unexpected benefits popped up. Babies and toddlers who signed showed less frustration, seemed to enjoy closer bonds with their parents, developed larger vocabularies early on, and even learned to read faster and more easily than their non-signing peers. I know, crazy right?

And it’s so simple! It is easy to teach your baby to sign. You don’t have to be fluent in American Sign Language. You can take it one sign at a time (or two, or three)!

A baby’s cognitive development is always ahead of his speech development. Many babies show proof of understanding what we say long before they are able to speak. Baby sign language can bridge this developmental gap. Signing with your baby will help her to communicate her thoughts long before she is able to verbalize them.

Many parents and caregivers choose to only teach a few signs, and that is fine! Some parents teach their babies to sign “diaper,” “down,” “eat,” and “milk” and stop there!

Imagine how much easier it would be if every time your baby wanted to nurse, she simply signed “milk.” She could sign before she cried. Wouldn’t that be great?

And many parents and caregivers teach their babies dozens of signs, so your baby could actually sign, “More applesauce please!”

Speaking of which, baby sign language is a great way to teach manners early. The signs for “please” and “thank you” are very easy to teach, and just as easy for your baby to learn.

A popular misconception is that babies who learn to sign will have no need for speaking, but this just isn’t the case. Study after study has shown the opposite to be true. Baby sign language encourages a baby to communicate, and fosters the skills and builds the confidence that he needs to do so.

It is easy to teach your baby sign language. The signs are all available for you at Baby Sign Language. If you are even thinking about it, you should give it a whirl. Your baby will thank you. She might even do it with a sign.

Guest post by Misty Weaver, Chief Editor, Baby Sign Language

Followers

Search This Blog

  © Blogger templates 'Sunshine' by Ourblogtemplates.com 2008

Back to TOP